By Yudhijit Bhattacharjee,
NY Times, March 17, 2012
SPEAKING two languages rather than just one has obvious practical benefits in an increasingly globalized world. But in recent years, scientists have begun to show that the advantages of bilingualism are even more fundamental than being able to converse with a wider range of people. Being bilingual, it turns out, makes you smarter. It can have a profound effect on your brain, improving cognitive skills not related to language and even shielding against dementia in old age.
SPEAKING two languages rather than just one has obvious practical benefits in an increasingly globalized world. But in recent years, scientists have begun to show that the advantages of bilingualism are even more fundamental than being able to converse with a wider range of people. Being bilingual, it turns out, makes you smarter. It can have a profound effect on your brain, improving cognitive skills not related to language and even shielding against dementia in old age.
This view of bilingualism
is remarkably different from the understanding of bilingualism through much of
the 20th century. Researchers, educators and policy makers long considered a
second language to be an interference, cognitively speaking, that hindered a
child’s academic and intellectual development.
They were not wrong about
the interference: there is ample evidence that in a bilingual’s brain both
language systems are active even when he is using only one language, thus
creating situations in which one system obstructs the other. But this
interference, researchers are finding out, isn’t so much a handicap as a
blessing in disguise. It forces the brain to resolve internal conflict, giving
the mind a workout that strengthens its cognitive muscles.
The collective evidence
from a number of studies suggests that the bilingual experience improves the
brain’s so-called executive function—a command system that directs the
attention processes that we use for planning, solving problems and performing
various other mentally demanding tasks. These processes include ignoring
distractions to stay focused, switching attention willfully from one thing to
another and holding information in mind—like remembering a sequence of
directions while driving.
“Bilinguals have to switch
languages quite often—you may talk to your father in one language and to your
mother in another language,” says Albert Costa, a researcher at the University
of Pompea Fabra in Spain. “It requires keeping track of changes around you in
the same way that we monitor our surroundings when driving.” In a study
comparing German-Italian bilinguals with Italian monolinguals on monitoring
tasks, Mr. Costa and his colleagues found that the bilingual subjects not only
performed better, but they also did so with less activity in parts of the brain
involved in monitoring, indicating that they were more efficient at it.
The bilingual experience
appears to influence the brain from infancy to old age (and there is reason to
believe that it may also apply to those who learn a second language later in
life).
In a recent study of 44
elderly Spanish-English bilinguals, scientists led by the neuropsychologist
Tamar Gollan of the University of California, San Diego, found that individuals
with a higher degree of bilingualism—measured through a comparative evaluation
of proficiency in each language—were more resistant than others to the onset of
dementia and other symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease: the higher the degree of
bilingualism, the later the age of onset.
Nobody ever doubted the
power of language. But who would have imagined that the words we hear and the
sentences we speak might be leaving such a deep imprint?
Yudhijit Bhattacharjee is a
staff writer at Science.
No comments:
Post a Comment